[1] Papaj D R, Prokopy R J. Ecological and evolutionary aspects of learning in phytophagous insects[J]. Annual review of entomology, 1989, 34(1): 315-350.
[2] 李月红, 刘树生. 植食性昆虫的学习行为[J]. 昆虫学报, 2004, 47(1): 106-116.
[3] Weiss M R. Innate colour preferences and flexible colour learning in the Pipevine swallowtail[J]. Animal Behaviour, 1997, 53(5): 1043-1052.
[4] Andersson S. Foraging responses in the butterflies Inachis io, Aglais urticae (Nymphalidae), and Gonepteryx rhamni (Pieridae) to floral scents[J]. Chemoecology, 2003, 13(1): 1-11.
[5] Honda K, Ômura H, Hayashi N. Identification of floral volatiles from Ligustrum japonicum that stimulate flower-visiting by cabbage butterfly, Pieris rapae[J]. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 1998, 24(12): 2167-2180.
[6] Kandori I, Yamaki T, Okuyama S, et al. Interspecific and intersexual learning rate differences in four butterfly species[J]. Journal of Experimental Biology, 2009, 212(23): 3810-3816.
[7] Van Emden H F, Eletherianos I, Rose J, et al. Aphid parasitoids detect that an alien plant was present nearby during their development[J]. Physiological entomology, 2002, 27(3): 199-205.
[8] Goulson D, Cory J S. Flower constancy and learning in foraging preferences of the green-veined white butterfly Pleris napi[J]. Ecological Entomology, 1993, 18(4): 315-320.
[9] Weiss M R. Associative colour learning in a nymphalid butterfly[J]. Ecological Entomology, 1995, 20(3): 298-301.
[10] 陈晓鸣,石 雷,周成理,等.中国观赏蝴蝶[M]. 北京:中国林业出版社,2008: 107-107.
[11] Gordon I J. Polymorphism of the tropical butterfly Danaus chrysippus (L.) in Africa[J]. Heredity, 1984, 53(3): 583-593.
[12] 王文明. 中华虎凤蝶和金斑蝶在燕山地区的生物学特性研究[D]. 南昌:南昌大学, 2011, 27-47.
[13] Boppré M, Petty R L, Schneider D, et al. Behaviorally mediated contacts between scent organs: Another prerequisite for pheromone production in Danaus chrysippus males (Lepidoptera)[J]. Journal of comparative physiology, 1978, 126(2): 97-103.
[14] Brower L P, Edmunds M, Moffitt C M. Cardenolide content and palatability of a population of Danaus chrysippus butterflies from West Africa[J]. Journal of Entomology Series A, General Entomology, 1975, 49(2): 183-196.
[15] 张雯雯. 蝴蝶蜜源及寄主植物挥发物成分的研究[D]. 北京:中国林业科学研究院, 2011, 60-60.
[16] Tang Y C, Zhou C L, Chen X M, et al. Visual and olfactory responses of seven butterfly species during foraging[J]. Journal of insect behavior, 2013, 26(3): 387-401.
[17] Raguso R A, Pichersky E. Floral volatiles from Clarkia breweri and C. concinna (Onagraceae): Recent evolution of floral scent and moth pollination[J]. Plant Systematics and Evolution, 1995, 194(1-2): 55-67.
[18] Andersson S, Dobson H E M. Behavioral foraging responses by the butterfly Heliconius melpomene to Lantana camara floral scent[J]. Journal of chemical ecology, 2003, 29(10): 2303-2318.
[19] Endler J A, Basolo A L. Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection[J]. Trends in ecology & evolution, 1998, 13(10): 415-420.
[20] Wright G A, Schiestl F P. The evolution of floral scent: the influence of olfactory learning by insect pollinators on the honest signalling of floral rewards[J]. Functional Ecology, 2009, 23(5): 841-851.
[21] Kandori I, Ohsaki N. The learning abilities of the white cabbage butterfly Pieris rapae foraging for flowers[J]. Researches on population ecology, 1996, 38(1): 111-117.